Jump to content

WikiJournal of Medicine/Dyslexia: Difference between revisions

From Wikiversity
Content deleted Content added
→‎Classification: not accurate - see editor comments on Discuss (Talk) page
Tag: 2017 source edit
Line 29: Line 29:
It is considered a [[w:cognitive disorder|cognitive disorder]], not a problem with [[w:intelligence|intelligence]]. However, [[w:Emotional and behavioral disorders|emotional problems]] often arise secondary to learning difficulties.<ref name="Campbell2009"/> Some published definitions propose causes, which usually cover a variety of reading skills and deficits, and difficulties with distinct causes rather than a single condition.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Lyytinen|first1=Heikki|title=In Search of a Science Based Application:Learning Tool for Reading Acquisition|journal=Scandinavian Journal of Psychology|date=November 2009|volume=50|issue=6|pages=668–675|pmid=19930268|doi=10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00791.x}}</ref> The [[w:National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke|National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke]] definition describes dyslexia as "difficulty with phonological processing (the manipulation of sounds), spelling, and/or rapid visual-verbal responding".<ref name="ninds1"/>
It is considered a [[w:cognitive disorder|cognitive disorder]], not a problem with [[w:intelligence|intelligence]]. However, [[w:Emotional and behavioral disorders|emotional problems]] often arise secondary to learning difficulties.<ref name="Campbell2009"/> Some published definitions propose causes, which usually cover a variety of reading skills and deficits, and difficulties with distinct causes rather than a single condition.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Lyytinen|first1=Heikki|title=In Search of a Science Based Application:Learning Tool for Reading Acquisition|journal=Scandinavian Journal of Psychology|date=November 2009|volume=50|issue=6|pages=668–675|pmid=19930268|doi=10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00791.x}}</ref> The [[w:National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke|National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke]] definition describes dyslexia as "difficulty with phonological processing (the manipulation of sounds), spelling, and/or rapid visual-verbal responding".<ref name="ninds1"/>


The British Dyslexia Association definition describes dyslexia as "a learning difficulty that primarily affects the skills involved in accurate and fluent word reading and spelling" and is characterized by "difficulties in phonological awareness, verbal memory and verbal processing speed".<ref name="PhillipsKelly2013">{{cite book|author1=Phillips, Sylvia|author2=Kelly, Kathleen|author3=Symes, Liz|title=Assessment of Learners with Dyslexic-Type Difficulties|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=7ZDCAQAAQBAJ&pg=PA7|year=2013|publisher=SAGE|isbn=978-1-4462-8704-0|page=7|deadurl=no|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20170109093024/https://books.google.com/books?id=7ZDCAQAAQBAJ&pg=PA7|archivedate=9 January 2017|df=dmy-all}}</ref> Phonological awareness enables one to identify, discriminate, remember, and manipulate the sound structures of language—[[w:phonemes|phonemes]], [[w:onsite-rime|onsite-rime]] segments, syllables, and words.
The British Dyslexia Association definition describes dyslexia as "a learning difficulty that primarily affects the skills involved in accurate and fluent word reading and spelling" and is characterized by "difficulties in phonological awareness, verbal memory and verbal processing speed".<ref name="PhillipsKelly2013">{{cite book|author1=Phillips, Sylvia|author2=Kelly, Kathleen|author3=Symes, Liz|title=Assessment of Learners with Dyslexic-Type Difficulties|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=7ZDCAQAAQBAJ&pg=PA7|year=2013|publisher=SAGE|isbn=978-1-4462-8704-0|page=7|deadurl=no|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20170109093024/https://books.google.com/books?id=7ZDCAQAAQBAJ&pg=PA7|archivedate=9 January 2017|df=dmy-all}}</ref> Phonological awareness enables one to identify, discriminate, remember, and manipulate the sound structures of language—[[w:phonemes|phonemes]], [[w:onsite-rime|onsite-rime]] segments, syllables, and words.


Acquired dyslexia, or alexia, may be caused by brain damage due to traumatic brain injury, [[w:stroke|stroke]] or [[w:cerebral atrophy|atrophy]].<ref name="MEDRS alexia">{{cite journal |last1=Woollams |first1=Anna M. |title=Connectionist neuropsychology: uncovering ultimate causes of acquired dyslexia |journal=Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences |volume=369 |issue=1634 |pages=20120398 |pmid=24324241 |pmc=3866427 |doi=10.1098/rstb.2012.0398 |date=9 December 2013 |url=http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/369/1634/20120398.long |accessdate=13 February 2015 |deadurl=no |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20151005055211/http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/369/1634/20120398.long |archivedate=5 October 2015 |df=dmy-all }}</ref><ref name="HeilmanMD2011s">{{cite book|last1=Heilman|first1=Kenneth M.|last2=Valenstein|first2=Edward|title=Clinical Neuropsychology|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=Qe70fMvSmOoC&pg=PA115|year=2011|publisher=Oxford University Press|isbn=978-0-19-538487-1|pages=115–116|deadurl=no|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20170109171557/https://books.google.com/books?id=Qe70fMvSmOoC&pg=PA115|archivedate=9 January 2017|df=dmy-all}}</ref> Forms of alexia include [[w:pure alexia|pure alexia]], [[w:surface dyslexia|surface dyslexia]], [[w:semantic dyslexia|semantic dyslexia]], [[w:phonological dyslexia|phonological dyslexia]], and [[w:deep dyslexia|deep dyslexia]].<ref name="SpiveyJoanisse2012n">{{cite book|author1=Spivey, Michael|author2=Joanisse, Marc|author3=McRae, Ken|title=The Cambridge Handbook of Psycholinguistics|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=niGsNQCn0AkC&pg=PA219|date=2012|publisher=Cambridge University Press|isbn=978-0-521-67792-9|pages=219–220|deadurl=no|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20170109194725/https://books.google.com/books?id=niGsNQCn0AkC&pg=PA219|archivedate=9 January 2017|df=dmy-all}}</ref>
Acquired dyslexia, or alexia, may be caused by brain damage due to traumatic brain injury, [[w:stroke|stroke]] or [[w:cerebral atrophy|atrophy]].<ref name="MEDRS alexia">{{cite journal |last1=Woollams |first1=Anna M. |title=Connectionist neuropsychology: uncovering ultimate causes of acquired dyslexia |journal=Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences |volume=369 |issue=1634 |pages=20120398 |pmid=24324241 |pmc=3866427 |doi=10.1098/rstb.2012.0398 |date=9 December 2013 |url=http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/369/1634/20120398.long |accessdate=13 February 2015 |deadurl=no |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20151005055211/http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/369/1634/20120398.long |archivedate=5 October 2015 |df=dmy-all }}</ref><ref name="HeilmanMD2011s">{{cite book|last1=Heilman|first1=Kenneth M.|last2=Valenstein|first2=Edward|title=Clinical Neuropsychology|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=Qe70fMvSmOoC&pg=PA115|year=2011|publisher=Oxford University Press|isbn=978-0-19-538487-1|pages=115–116|deadurl=no|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20170109171557/https://books.google.com/books?id=Qe70fMvSmOoC&pg=PA115|archivedate=9 January 2017|df=dmy-all}}</ref> Forms of alexia include [[w:pure alexia|pure alexia]], [[w:surface dyslexia|surface dyslexia]], [[w:semantic dyslexia|semantic dyslexia]], [[w:phonological dyslexia|phonological dyslexia]], and [[w:deep dyslexia|deep dyslexia]].<ref name="SpiveyJoanisse2012n">{{cite book|author1=Spivey, Michael|author2=Joanisse, Marc|author3=McRae, Ken|title=The Cambridge Handbook of Psycholinguistics|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=niGsNQCn0AkC&pg=PA219|date=2012|publisher=Cambridge University Press|isbn=978-0-521-67792-9|pages=219–220|deadurl=no|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20170109194725/https://books.google.com/books?id=niGsNQCn0AkC&pg=PA219|archivedate=9 January 2017|df=dmy-all}}</ref>

Revision as of 17:12, 6 October 2019

WikiJournal of Medicine
Open access • Publication charge free • Public peer review • Wikipedia-integrated

WikiJournal of Medicine is an open-access, free-to-publish, Wikipedia-integrated academic journal for Medical and Biomedical topics. <seo title=" WJM, WikiJMed, Wiki.J.Med., WikiJMed, Wikiversity Journal of Medicine, WikiJournal Medicine, Wikipedia Medicine, Wikipedia medical journal, WikiMed, Wikimedicine, Wikimedical, Medicine, Biomedicine, Free to publish, Open access, Open-access, Non-profit, online journal, Public peer review "/>

<meta name='citation_doi' value='10.15347/WJM/2019.005'>

Article information

Submitting author: Ozzie Anis[i] 
Additional contributors: Wikipedia community

See author information ▼

Abstract

Dyslexia, also known as reading disorder, is characterized by trouble with reading despite the presence of otherwise spared cognitive and achievement abilities.[1][2] Dyslexia manifests on a continuum of severity—it is a dimensional disorder.[3][4] Problems may include difficulties in spelling words, reading quickly, writing words, "sounding out" words (subvocalization), pronouncing words when reading aloud, and understanding what one reads.[4][5] Teachers often identify these difficulties before parents or others. People with this disorder have a normal desire to learn.[4]

Dyslexia is believed to be caused by both genetic and environmental factors, and their interaction.[6] Some cases run in families.[4] People with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and individuals who experience difficulties with numbers, exhibit higher rates of dyslexia than the general population. [6] Dyslexia that develops subsequent to a traumatic brain injury, stroke, or dementia is called acquired dyslexia or alexia.[1] The underlying mechanisms of dyslexia are problems within the brain's language processing.[4] Dyslexia is diagnosed through a series of tests of memory, spelling, vision, and reading skills.[7] Dyslexia is separate from reading difficulties caused by hearing or vision problems or by insufficient teaching or opportunity to learn.[6]

Treatment involves adjusting teaching methods to meet the person's needs.[1] While not curing the underlying problem, it may decrease the degree or impact of symptoms.[8] Treatments targeting vision are not effective.[9] Dyslexia is the most common learning disability and occurs in all areas of the world.[6][10] It affects 3–7% of the population,[6][11] however, up to 20% of the general population may have some degree of symptoms.[12] While dyslexia is more often diagnosed in men,[6] it has been suggested that it affects men and women equally.[10]


Classification

It is considered a cognitive disorder, not a problem with intelligence. However, emotional problems often arise secondary to learning difficulties.[13] Some published definitions propose causes, which usually cover a variety of reading skills and deficits, and difficulties with distinct causes rather than a single condition.[14] The National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke definition describes dyslexia as "difficulty with phonological processing (the manipulation of sounds), spelling, and/or rapid visual-verbal responding".[1]

The British Dyslexia Association definition describes dyslexia as "a learning difficulty that primarily affects the skills involved in accurate and fluent word reading and spelling" and is characterized by "difficulties in phonological awareness, verbal memory and verbal processing speed".[15] Phonological awareness enables one to identify, discriminate, remember, and manipulate the sound structures of language—phonemes, onsite-rime segments, syllables, and words.[16][17]

Acquired dyslexia, or alexia, may be caused by brain damage due to traumatic brain injury, stroke or atrophy.[18][19] Forms of alexia include pure alexia, surface dyslexia, semantic dyslexia, phonological dyslexia, and deep dyslexia.[20]

Clinical and educational diagnosis uses standardized testing; there is no neuroimaging test that can diagnose dyslexia at present. There are a variety of different classification models for specific learning disabilities. Three that have recently been the focus of research include:

  • Concordance-Discordance Method:[21] The Concordance-Discordance method compares pairs of scores, testing whether the difference is larger than could be attributed to imprecision of the scores. The model tests at least three pairs of scores within a person, looking for a discrepancy between two cognitive scores, a difference between the higher cognitive score (the cognitive “strength”) and the reading achievement score (with the reading score being significantly lower) no difference between the reading score and the lower cognitive score.[21][22] This model is more conservative than the “simple difference” method, where a reading achievement score one standard deviation lower (15 points on most tests) would be considered indicative of a reading disability. The simple difference method ignores the fact that neither component test is perfectly reliable.[23]
  • Discrepancy/Consistency Method:[24] This model compares the reading ability to general cognitive ability, and tests whether the difference between scores is large enough to be statistically reliable. Some experts argue for using tests where the cognitive and achievement tests were normed in the same sample, as this gives a more accurate estimate of the degree of correlation between scores, as well as avoiding artifacts such as sampling differences or the Flynn effect when scales from different test batteries are compared.[24]
  • Cross Battery Assessment:[25][26] The cross-battery model also looks for patterns of strengths and weaknesses in cognitive and achievement scores as a way of detecting learning disability. The “cross-battery” aspect combines subtests from multiple cognitive ability measures to so that there are usually at least four different scales converging to measure an aspect of functioning. For example, most versions of the Wechsler include two subtests for working memory. In a cross-battery approach, a psychologist might add two other working memory subtests from different measures to create an estimate of working memory based on four scores. Unlike the other discrepancy methods, cross-battery assessment also considers whether the reading ability is below average for the youth’s age, defined as a standard score of <85.[25]

All of these approaches conceptually are testing whether the reading performance is below what would be expected based on the person’s general cognitive functioning. Limitations of these approaches include (a) poor agreement about which cases have a learning disability,[22][23] (b) low reliability both in simulation studies[27] and when testing is repeated with real youths,[28][27] and (c) inconsistent evidence of validity of the definitions when predicting grades, graduation, or other criteria of interest.[29][23]

Signs and symptoms

In early childhood, symptoms that correlate with a later diagnosis of dyslexia include delayed onset of speech and a lack of phonological awareness, as well as being easily distracted by background noise.[9] A common myth closely associates dyslexia with mirror writing and reading letters or words backwards.[30] These behaviors are seen in many children as they learn to read and write, and are not considered to be defining characteristics of dyslexia.[9]

School-age children with dyslexia may exhibit signs of difficulty in identifying or generating rhyming words, or counting the number of syllables in words – both of which depend on phonological awareness.[31] They may also show difficulty in segmenting words into individual sounds or may blend sounds when producing words, indicating reduced phonemic awareness.[32] Difficulties with word retrieval or naming things is also associated with dyslexia.[33]:647 People with dyslexia are commonly poor spellers, a feature sometimes called dysorthographia or dysgraphia, which depends on orthographic coding.[9]

Problems persist into adolescence and adulthood and may include difficulties with summarizing stories, memorization, reading aloud, or learning foreign languages. Adults with dyslexia can often read with good comprehension, though they tend to read more slowly than others without a learning difficulty and perform worse in spelling tests or when reading nonsense words – a measure of phonological awareness.[34]

Associated conditions

Dyslexia is often accompanied by several learning disabilities, but it is unclear whether they share underlying neurological causes.[35] These associated disabilities include:

  • Dysgraphia – A disorder involving difficulties with writing or typing, sometimes due to problems with eye–hand coordination; it also can impede direction- or sequence-oriented processes, such as tying knots or carrying out repetitive tasks.[36] In dyslexia, dysgraphia is often multifactorial, due to impaired letter-writing automaticity, organizational and elaborative difficulties, and impaired visual word forming, which makes it more difficult to retrieve the visual picture of words required for spelling.[36]
  • Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) – A disorder characterized by problems sustaining attention, hyperactivity, or acting impulsively.[37] Dyslexia and ADHD commonly occur together.[11][38][39] Roughly 15%[9] or 12–24% of people with dyslexia have ADHD;[40] and up to 35% of people with ADHD have dyslexia.[9]
  • Auditory processing disorder – A listening disorder that affects the ability to process auditory information.[41][42] This can lead to problems with auditory memory and auditory sequencing. Many people with dyslexia have auditory processing problems, and may develop their own logographic cues to compensate for this type of deficit. Some research suggests that auditory processing skills could be the primary shortfall in dyslexia.[43][44]
  • Developmental coordination disorder – A neurological condition characterized by difficulty in carrying out routine tasks involving balance, fine-motor control, kinesthetic coordination, difficulty in the use of speech sounds, problems with short-term memory, and organization.[45]

Causes

width (default = 250px)

 Inferior parietal lobule - superior view animation


Anatomography/Attribution-ShareAlike 2.1 Japan (CC BY-SA 2.1 JP)

Researchers have been trying to find the neurobiological basis of dyslexia since the condition was first identified in 1881.[46][47] For example, some have tried to associate the common problem among dyslexics of not being able to see letters clearly to abnormal development of their visual nerve cells.[48]

Neuroanatomy

Modern neuroimaging techniques, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET), have shown a correlation between both functional and structural differences in the brains of children with reading difficulties.[49] Some dyslexics show less electrical activation in parts of the left hemisphere of the brain involved with reading, such as the inferior frontal gyrus, inferior parietal lobule, and the middle and ventral temporal cortex.[43] Over the past decade, brain activation studies using PET to study language have produced a breakthrough in the understanding of the neural basis of language. Neural bases for the visual lexicon and for auditory verbal short-term memory components have been proposed,[50] with some implication that the observed neural manifestation of developmental dyslexia is task-specific (i.e., functional rather than structural). fMRIs of people with dyslexia indicate an interactive role of the cerebellum and cerebral cortex as well as other brain structures in reading.[51][52]

The cerebellar theory of dyslexia proposes that impairment of cerebellum-controlled muscle movement affects the formation of words by the tongue and facial muscles, resulting in the fluency problems that some people with dyslexia experience. The cerebellum is also involved in the automatization of some tasks, such as reading.[53] The fact that some dyslexic children have motor task and balance impairments could be consistent with a cerebellar role in their reading difficulties. However, the cerebellar theory has not been supported by controlled research studies.[54]

Genetics

Research into potential genetic causes of dyslexia has its roots in post-autopsy examination of the brains of people with dyslexia.[48] Observed anatomical differences in the language centers of such brains include microscopic cortical malformations known as ectopias, and more rarely, vascular micro-malformations, and microgyrus -- a smaller than usual size for the gyrus.[55] The previously cited studies and others[56] suggest that abnormal cortical development, presumed to occur before or during the sixth month of fetal brain development, may have caused the abnormalities. Abnormal cell formations in dyslexics have also been reported in non-language cerebral and subcortical brain structures.[57] Several genes have been associated with dyslexia, including DCDC2 and KIAA0319 on chromosome 6,[58] and DYX1C1 on chromosome 15.[59]

Gene–environment interaction

The contribution of gene–environment interaction to reading disability has been intensely studied using twin studies, which estimate the proportion of variance associated with a person's environment and the proportion associated with their genes. Both environmental and genetic factors appear to contribute to reading development. Studies examining the influence of environmental factors such as parental education[60] and teaching quality[61] have determined that genetics have greater influence in supportive, rather than less optimal, environments.[62] However, more optimal conditions may just allow those genetic risk factors to account for more of the variance in outcome because the environmental risk factors have been minimized.[62]

As environment plays a large role in learning and memory, it is likely that epigenetic modifications play an important role in reading ability. Measures of gene expression, histone modifications, and methylation in the human periphery are used to study epigenetic processes; however, all of these have limitations in the extrapolation of results for application to the human brain.[63][64]

Language

The orthographic complexity of a language directly affects how difficult it is to learn to read it.[65]:266 English and French have comparatively "deep" phonemic orthographies within the Latin alphabet writing system, with complex structures employing spelling patterns on several levels: letter-sound correspondence, syllables, and morphemes.[66]:421 Languages such as Spanish, Italian and Finnish have mostly alphabetic orthographies, which primarily employ letter-sound correspondence – so-called "shallow" orthographies – which makes them easier to learn for people with dyslexia.[65]:266 Logographic writing systems, such as Chinese characters, have extensive symbol use; and these also pose problems for dyslexic learners.[67]

Pathophysiology

Most people who are right-hand dominant have the left hemisphere of their brain specialize more in language processing. In terms of the mechanism of dyslexia, fMRI studies suggest that this specialization may be less pronounced or even absent in cases with dyslexia. Additionally, anatomical differences in the corpus callosum, the bundle of nerve fibers that connects the left and right hemispheres, have been linked to dyslexia via different studies.[68] Data via diffusion tensor MRI indicate changes in connectivity or in gray matter density in areas related to reading/language. Finally, the left inferior frontal gyrus has shown differences in phonological processing in dyslexics.[68]

Neurophysiological and imaging procedures are being used to ascertain phenotypic characteristics in dyslexics, thus identifying the effects of certain genes.[69]

Dual route theory

The dual-route theory of reading aloud was first described in the early 1970s.[70] This theory suggests that two separate mental mechanisms, or cognitive routes, are involved in reading aloud.[71] One mechanism is the lexical route, which is the process whereby skilled readers can recognize known words by sight alone, through a "dictionary" lookup procedure.[72] The other mechanism is the nonlexical or sublexical route, which is the process whereby the reader can "sound out" a written word.[72][73] This is done by identifying the word's constituent parts (letters, phonemes, graphemes) and applying knowledge of how these parts are associated with each other, for example, how a string of neighboring letters sound together.[70] The dual-route system could explain the different rates of dyslexia occurrence between different languages (e.g., the consistency of phonological rules in the Spanish language could account for the fact that Spanish-speaking children show a higher level of performance in non-word reading, when compared to English-speakers).[65][74]

Diagnosis

There are a wide range of tests that are used in clinical and educational settings to evaluate the possibility that a person might have dyslexia.[75] If initial testing suggests that a person might have dyslexia, such tests are often followed up with a full diagnostic assessment to determine the extent and nature of the disorder.[76] Some tests can be administered by a teacher or computer; others require specialized training and are given by psychologists.[77] Some test results indicate how to carry out teaching strategies.[77][78] Because a variety of different cognitive, behavioral, emotional, and environmental factors all could contribute to difficultly learning to read, a comprehensive evaluation should consider these different possibilities. These tests and observations can include:[79]

  • General measures of cognitive ability, such as the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities, or Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales. Low general cognitive ability would make reading more difficult. Cognitive ability measures also often try to measure different cognitive cognitive processes, such as verbal ability, nonverbal and spatial reasoning, working memory, and processing speed. There are different versions of these tests for different age groups. Almost all of these require additional training to give and score correctly, and are done by psychologists. According to Mather and Schneider (2015), a confirmatory profile and/or pattern of scores on cognitive tests confirming or ruling-out reading disorder has not yet been identified.[80]
  • Screening or evaluation for mental health conditions: Parents and teachers can complete rating scales or behavior checklists to gather information about emotional and behavioral functioning for the youth. Many checklists have similar versions for parents, teachers, and youths old enough to read reasonably well (often 11 years and older) to complete. Examples include the Behavioral Assessment System for Children, and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. All of these have nationally representative norms, making it possible to compare the level of symptoms to what would be typical for the youth's age and biological sex. Other checklists link more specifically to psychiatric diagnoses, such as the Vanderbilt ADHD Rating Scales or the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED). Screening uses brief tools that are designed to catch cases with a disorder, but they often get false positive scores for people who do not have the disorder. Screeners should be followed up by a more accurate test or diagnostic interview as a result. Depressive disorders and anxiety disorders are two-three times higher in dyslexics, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is more common, as well.[81][82][83][84]
  • Review of academic achievement and skills: Average spelling/reading ability for a dyslexic is a percentage ranking <16, well below normal. In additional to reviewing grades and teacher notes, standardized test results are helpful in evaluating progress. These include group administered tests, such as the Iowa Tests of Educational Development, that a teacher may give to a group or whole classroom of youths at the same time. They also could include individually administered tests of achievement, such as the Wide Range Achievement Test, or the Woodcock-Johnson (which also includes a set of achievement tests). The individually administered tests again require more specialized training.[85][86][87]

Definition

The condition of dyslexia is defined simply as an inability to read in the context of normal intelligence. Sometimes it is helpful to distinguish between developmental dyslexia (a learning disorder) and acquired dyslexia (loss of the ability to read caused by brain damage). The International Classification of Diseases (ICD 10), the World Health Organization's manual of medical diagnosis used in most of the world, includes separate diagnoses for "developmental dyslexia" (81.0)[88] and for "dyslexia and alexia" (48.0).[89] The new update to the ICD, version 11, retains these. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of the American Psychiatric Association, DSM 5, the manual of psychiatric diagnosis more widely used in the United States, does not specifically define dyslexia. The DSM-5 authors justified this decision by stating that "the many definitions of dyslexia and dyscalculia meant those terms would not be useful as disorder names or in the diagnostic criteria". Instead DSM-5 subsume dyslexia in a category called specific learning disorders.[90]

Types of dyslexia

A high-level distinction can be made between developmental dyslexias (also referred to as congenital dyslexias or specific developmental dyslexias) versus acquired dyslexia (also referred to as traumatic dyslexia).[91] Although it is likely that subtypes of dyslexia exist, their implications for treatment planning are not yet well understood, and interventions that claim to work for specific subtypes have not accrued clear support yet.[92]

Subtypes

Central dyslexias

Central dyslexias include the following: surface dyslexia, semantic dyslexia, phonological dyslexia, and deep dyslexia.[18][93]

Surface dyslexia

In surface dyslexia, words with regular pronunciations (highly consistent with their spelling, e.g., mint) are read more accurately than words with irregular pronunciation, such as colonel.[94] Difficulty distinguishing homophones is a diagnostic feature used to help identify some forms of surface dyslexia. Surface dyslexia is usually accompanied by surface agraphia and fluent aphasia.[95] Acquired surface dyslexia arises when a previously literate person experiences brain damage, which results in pronunciation errors that indicate impairment of the lexical route.[18][96][97]

Phonological dyslexia

 Dyslexics overuse this area associated with speech.[98]


Database Center for Life Science(DBCLS)/CC-BY-SA-2.1-jp

In phonological dyslexia, sufferers can read familiar words but have difficulty with unfamiliar words, such as invented pseudo-words.[99] Phonological dyslexia is associated with lesions in the parts of the brain supplied with blood by the middle cerebral artery. The superior temporal lobe is often also involved. Dyslexics often compensate by overusing a front-brain region called Broca's area, which is associated with aspects of language and speech.[100] The Lindamood Phoneme Sequencing Program (LiPS) is designed to treat phonological dyslexia.[101] It uses a three-way sensory feedback process, using auditory, visual, and oral skills to learn to recognize words and word patterns. Case studies with a total of three patients found a significant improvement in spelling and reading ability after using LiPS.[102]

Human syntax--the arrangement of words and phrases--is established in the posterior region of Broca's area, via white matter links to the human temporal cortex. This neurological circuit is important in human language, particularly as it arranges words and phrases.[103]

Wernicke's area is located in the posterior left sylvian fissure, anatomically speaking. Functional neuroimaging methods indicate that Wernicke's area is important in phonologic retrieval in normal-functioning individuals.[104] During rhyming tasks, Wernicke's area was not activated while Broca's was activated among cases with dyslexia.[105]

Deep dyslexia

Individuals with deep dyslexia experience both semantic paralexia (para-dyslexia) and phonological dyslexia, causing the person to read a word and then say a related meaning instead of the denoted meaning.[106] Deep dyslexia is associated with clear phonological processing impairments.[18] Deep dyslexia can be caused by widespread damage to the brain that often includes the left hemisphere.[107] The "continuum" hypothesis claims that deep dyslexia develops from phonological dyslexia.[108]

Peripheral dyslexias

Peripheral dyslexias have been described as affecting the visual analysis of letters as a result of brain injury.[109] Hemianopsia, a visual field loss on the left/right side of the vertical midline, is associated with this condition.[110][111]

Pure dyslexia

Pure, or phonologically-based,[112] dyslexia, also known as agnosic dyslexia, dyslexia without agraphia, and pure word blindness, is dyslexia due to difficulty in recognizing written sequences of letters (such as words), or sometimes even letters. It is considered '"pure" because it is not accompanied by other significant language-related impairments. Pure dyslexia does not affect speech, handwriting style, language or comprehension impairments.[113] Pure dyslexia is caused by lesions on the visual word form area (VWFA). The VWFA is composed of the left lateral occipital sulcus and is activated during reading. A lesion in the VWFA stops transmission between the visual cortex and the left angular gyrus. It can also be caused by a lesion involving the left occipital lobe or the splenium. It is usually accompanied by a homonymous hemianopsia in the right side of the visual field.[109] Multiple oral re-reading (MOR) is a treatment for pure dyslexia.[114] It is considered a top-down processing technique in which affected individuals read and reread texts a predetermined number of times or until reading speed or accuracy improves a predetermined amount.[115]

Hemianopic dyslexia

Hemianopic dyslexia is commonly considered to derive from visual field loss due to damage to the primary visual cortex.[116] Sufferers may complain of abnormally slow reading but are able to read individual words normally. This is the most common form of peripheral alexia, and the form with the best evidence of effective treatments.[117]

Neglect dyslexia

In neglect dyslexia, some letters, most commonly those at the beginning or left side of a word, are skipped or misread during reading.[118] This alexia is associated with right parietal lesions. The use of prism glasses has been shown to mitigate this condition substantially.[119]

Management

Through the use of compensation strategies, therapy and educational support, dyslexic individuals can learn to read and write.[120] There are techniques and technical aids that help to manage or conceal symptoms of the disorder.[121] Reducing stress and anxiety can sometimes improve written comprehension.[122] For dyslexia intervention with alphabet-writing systems, the fundamental aim is to increase a child's awareness of correspondences between graphemes (letters) and phonemes (sounds), and to relate these to reading and spelling by teaching how sounds blend into words. Reinforced collateral training focused on reading and spelling may yield longer-lasting gains than oral phonological training alone.[123] Early intervention can be successful in reducing reading failure.[124]

There is some evidence that the use of specially-tailored fonts may help with dyslexia.[125] These fonts, which include Dyslexie, OpenDyslexic, and Lexie Readable, were created based on the idea that many of the letters of the Latin alphabet are visually similar and may, therefore, confuse people with dyslexia. Dyslexie and OpenDyslexic both put emphasis on making each letter more distinctive in order to be more easily identified.[126] The benefits, however, might largely be due to the added spacing between words.[127]

There have been many studies conducted regarding intervention in dyslexia. A meta-analysis found that there was functional activation as a result.[128] There is no evidence showing that music education significantly improves dyslexic adolescents' reading skills.[129]

Prognosis

Dyslexic children require special instruction for word analysis and spelling from an early age.[130] While there are fonts that may help people with dyslexia better understand writing, this might simply be due to the added spacing between words.[125][127] The prognosis, generally speaking, is positive for individuals who are identified in childhood and receive support from friends and family.[1] The New York educational system (NYED) indicates "a daily uninterrupted 90 minute block of instruction in reading", furthermore "instruction in phonemic awareness, phonics, vocabulary development, reading fluency" so as to improve the individuals reading ability[131].

Epidemiology

The percentage of people with dyslexia is unknown, but it has been estimated to be as low as 5% and as high as 17% of the population.[132] While it is diagnosed more often in males,[6] some believe that it affects males and females equally.

There are different definitions of dyslexia used throughout the world, but despite significant differences in writing systems, dyslexia occurs in different populations.[133] Dyslexia is not limited to difficulty in converting letters to sounds, and Chinese dyslexics may have difficulty converting Chinese characters into their meanings.[134][135] The Chinese vocabulary uses logographic, monographic, non-alphabet writing where one character can represent an individual phoneme.[136]

The phonological-processing hypothesis attempts to explain why dyslexia occurs in a wide variety of languages. Furthermore, the relationship between phonological capacity and reading appears to be influenced by orthography.[137]

History

Dyslexia was clinically described by Oswald Berkhan in 1881,[46] but the term dyslexia was coined in 1887 by Rudolf Berlin, an ophthalmologist in Stuttgart.[138] He used the term to refer to the case of a young boy who had severe difficulty learning to read and write, despite showing typical intelligence and physical abilities in all other respects.[139] In 1896, W. Pringle Morgan, a British physician from Seaford, East Sussex, published a description of a reading-specific learning disorder in a report to the British Medical Journal titled "Congenital Word Blindness".[140] The distinction between phonological versus surface types of dyslexia is only descriptive, and without any etiological assumption as to the underlying brain mechanisms. However, studies have alluded to potential differences due to variation in performance.[141]

Research and social perceptions

 Writing Systems


CC-BY-SA-4.0

Most currently available dyslexia research relates to alphabetic writing systems, and especially to European languages.[142] However, substantial research is also available regarding dyslexics who speak Arabic, Chinese, Hebrew, or other languages.[143] The outward expression of individuals with reading disability and regular poor readers is the same in some respects[144].

As is the case with any disorder, society often makes an assessment based on incomplete information. Before the 1980s, dyslexia was thought to be a consequence of education, rather than a neurological disability. As a result, society often misjudges those with the disorder.[122] There is also sometimes a workplace stigma and negative attitude towards those with dyslexia.[145] If a dyslexic's instructors lack the necessary training to support a child with the condition, there is often a negative effect on the student's learning participation.[146]

References

  1. 1.0 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 "NINDS Dyslexia Information Page". National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke. National Institutes of Health. 11 September 2015. Archived from the original on 27 July 2016. Retrieved 27 July 2016. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (help)
  2. Siegel, LS (November 2006). "Perspectives on dyslexia". Paediatrics & Child Health 11 (9): 581–7. doi:10.1093/pch/11.9.581. PMID 19030329. PMC 2528651. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2528651/. 
  3. Snowling, Margaret J. Dyslexia: A Very Short Introduction. Oxford University Press, 2019. ISBN 9780192550422
  4. 4.0 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 "What are reading disorders?". National Institutes of Health. Archived from the original on 2 April 2015. Retrieved 15 March 2015. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (help)
  5. "What are the symptoms of reading disorders?". National Institutes of Health. Archived from the original on 2 April 2015. Retrieved 15 March 2015. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (help)
  6. 6.0 6.1 6.2 6.3 6.4 6.5 6.6 Peterson, Robin L.; Pennington, Bruce F. (May 2012). "Developmental dyslexia". Lancet 379 (9830): 1997–2007. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60198-6. PMID 22513218. PMC 3465717. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3465717/. 
  7. "How are reading disorders diagnosed?". National Institutes of Health. Archived from the original on 2 April 2015. Retrieved 15 March 2015. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (help)
  8. "What are common treatments for reading disorders?". National Institutes of Health. Archived from the original on 2 April 2015. Retrieved 15 March 2015. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (help)
  9. 9.0 9.1 9.2 9.3 9.4 9.5 Handler, SM; Fierson, WM; Section on, Ophthalmology; Council on Children with, Disabilities; American Academy of, Ophthalmology; American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and, Strabismus; American Association of Certified, Orthoptists (March 2011). "Learning disabilities, dyslexia, and vision.". Pediatrics 127 (3): e818–56. doi:10.1542/peds.2010-3670. PMID 21357342. 
  10. 10.0 10.1 Umphred, Darcy Ann; Lazaro, Rolando T.; Roller, Margaret; Burton, Gordon (2013). Neurological Rehabilitation. Elsevier Health Sciences. p. 383. ISBN 978-0-323-26649-9. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109173020/https://books.google.com/books?id=lVJPAQAAQBAJ&pg=PA383. 
  11. 11.0 11.1 Kooij, J. J. Sandra (2013). Adult ADHD diagnostic assessment and treatment (3rd ed.). London: Springer. p. 83. ISBN 9781447141389. Archived from the original on 30 April 2016. https://web.archive.org/web/20160430012545/https://books.google.com/books?id=JM_awX-mSPoC&pg=PA83. 
  12. "How many people are affected by/at risk for reading disorders?". National Institutes of Health. Archived from the original on 2 April 2015. Retrieved 15 March 2015. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (help)
  13. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Campbell2009
  14. Lyytinen, Heikki (November 2009). "In Search of a Science Based Application:Learning Tool for Reading Acquisition". Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 50 (6): 668–675. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00791.x. PMID 19930268. 
  15. Phillips, Sylvia; Kelly, Kathleen; Symes, Liz (2013). Assessment of Learners with Dyslexic-Type Difficulties. SAGE. p. 7. ISBN 978-1-4462-8704-0. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109093024/https://books.google.com/books?id=7ZDCAQAAQBAJ&pg=PA7. 
  16. Stahl, Steven A.; Murray, Bruce A. (1994). "Defining phonological awareness and its relationship to early reading". Journal of Educational Psychology. pp. 221–234. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.86.2.221. Retrieved 6 October 2019.
  17. "Phonological Awareness and Phonemic Perception in 4-Year-Old Children With Delayed Expressive Phonology Skills". American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 12 (4): 463–471. 1 November 2003. doi:10.1044/1058-0360(2003/092). https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2003/092). Retrieved 6 October 2019. 
  18. 18.0 18.1 18.2 18.3 Woollams, Anna M. (9 December 2013). "Connectionist neuropsychology: uncovering ultimate causes of acquired dyslexia". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 369 (1634): 20120398. doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0398. PMID 24324241. PMC 3866427. Archived from the original on 5 October 2015. http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/369/1634/20120398.long. Retrieved 13 February 2015. 
  19. Heilman, Kenneth M.; Valenstein, Edward (2011). Clinical Neuropsychology. Oxford University Press. pp. 115–116. ISBN 978-0-19-538487-1. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109171557/https://books.google.com/books?id=Qe70fMvSmOoC&pg=PA115. 
  20. Spivey, Michael; Joanisse, Marc; McRae, Ken (2012). The Cambridge Handbook of Psycholinguistics. Cambridge University Press. pp. 219–220. ISBN 978-0-521-67792-9. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109194725/https://books.google.com/books?id=niGsNQCn0AkC&pg=PA219. 
  21. 21.0 21.1 Hale, J.B.; Fiorello, C.A.; Miller, J.A.; Wenrich, K.; Teodori, A.; Henzel, J.N. (2008). "WISC-IV interpretation for specific learning disabilities and intervention: A cognitive hypothesis testing approach". WISC-IV Clinical Assessment and Intervention (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Elsevier. pp. 109–171. 
  22. 22.0 22.1 Stuebing, Karla K.; Fletcher, Jack M.; Branum-Martin, Lee; Francis, David J. (2012). "Evaluation of the Technical Adequacy of Three Methods for Identifying Specific Learning Disabilities Based on Cognitive Discrepancies". School Psychology Review 41 (1): 3–22. ISSN 0279-6015. PMID 23060685. PMC 3466817. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3466817/. 
  23. 23.0 23.1 23.2 McGill, Ryan J.; Styck, Kara M.; Palomares, Ronald S.; Hass, Michael R. (7 December 2015). "Critical Issues in Specific Learning Disability Identification". Learning Disability Quarterly 39 (3): 159–170. doi:10.1177/0731948715618504. 
  24. 24.0 24.1 Naglieri, Jack A.; Das, J.P. (1997). Cognitive Assessment System: Interpretive Handbook. Rolling Meadows, IL: Riverside Publishing. ISBN 978-0782893731. OCLC 967493665. 
  25. 25.0 25.1 Flanagan, Dawn P. (2007). Essentials of cross-battery assessment. Ortiz, Samuel O., 1958-, Alfonso, Vincent C. (2nd ed.). Hoboken, N.J.: John Wiley & Sons. ISBN 9780471757719. OCLC 76064575. 
  26. McGrew, D. P. & , K. S (1997). "A cross-battery to assessing and interpreting cognitive abilities: Narrowing the gap between practice and cognitive science". In Flanagan, Dawn P.; Harrison, Patti L.. Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues. New York: The Guilford Press. pp. 314–325. ISBN 978-1593851255. OCLC 34753160. 
  27. 27.0 27.1 Macmann, Gregg M.; Barnett, David W. (23/1997). "Myth of the master detective: Reliability of interpretations for Kaufman's "intelligent testing" approach to the WISC–III.". School Psychology Quarterly 12 (3): 197–234. doi:10.1037/h0088959. ISSN 1939-1560. 
  28. Watkins, Marley W.; Canivez, Gary L. (2004-6). "Temporal stability of WISC-III subtest composite: strengths and weaknesses". Psychological Assessment 16 (2): 133–138. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.16.2.133. ISSN 1040-3590. PMID 15222809. 
  29. Flanagan, Dawn P.; Harrison, Patti L. (2012). Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues (in en). Guilford Press. ISBN 9781609189952. https://books.google.com/?id=IUiC8moHcTsC&printsec=frontcover&dq=Contemporary+intellectual+assessment:+Theories,+tests,+and+issue#v=onepage&q=Cross%20battery&f=false. Retrieved 22 September 2019. pg 459
  30. Lilienfeld, Scott O.; Lynn, Steven Jay; Ruscio, John; Beyerstein, Barry L. (15 September 2011). 50 Great Myths of Popular Psychology: Shattering Widespread Misconceptions about Human Behavior. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 88–89. ISBN 978-1-4443-6074-5. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109130327/https://books.google.com/books?id=8DlS0gfO_QUC&pg=PT88. Retrieved 19 May 2016. 
  31. "Dyslexia and Related Disorders" (PDF). Alabama Dyslexia Association. International Dyslexia Association. January 2003. Archived from the original (PDF) on 4 March 2016. Retrieved 29 April 2015. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (help)
  32. Peer, Lindsay; Reid, Gavin (2014). Multilingualism, Literacy and Dyslexia. Routledge. p. 219. ISBN 978-1-136-60899-5. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109204808/https://books.google.com/books?id=-aoABAAAQBAJ&pg=PA219. 
  33. Shaywitz, Sally E.; Shaywitz, Bennett A. (2013). "Chapter 34 Making a Hidden Disability Visible: What Has Been Learned from Neurobiological Studies of Dyslexia". Handbook of Learning Disabilities (2 ed.). Guilford Press. ISBN 978-1-4625-0856-3. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://books.google.com/books?id=oakQfUuutVwC&pg=PA647. 
  34. Jarrad, Lum (October 2013). "Procedural learning is impaired in dyslexia: evidence from a meta-analysis of serial reaction time studies". Research of Developmental Disabilities 34 (10): 3460–76. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.07.017. PMID 23920029. PMC 3784964. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3784964/. 
  35. Nicolson, R. I.; Fawcett, A. J. (September 2009). "Dyslexia, dysgraphia, procedural learning and the cerebellum". Cortex 47 (1): 117–27. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.016. PMID 19818437. 
  36. 36.0 36.1 Reynolds, Cecil R.; Fletcher-Janzen, Elaine (2 January 2007). Encyclopedia of Special Education. John Wiley & Sons. p. 771. ISBN 978-0-471-67798-7. 
  37. "Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder". NIH: National Institute of Mental Health. March 2016. Archived from the original on 23 July 2016. Retrieved 26 July 2016. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (help)
  38. Comer, Ronald (2011). Psychology Around Us. RR Donnelley. p. 1. ISBN 978-0-471-38519-6. Archived from the original on 4 June 2016. https://web.archive.org/web/20160604000711/https://books.google.com/books?id=ySIc1BcPJu8C&pg=RA1-PA233. 
  39. Germanò, E; Gagliano, A; Curatolo, P (2010). "Comorbidity of ADHD and Dyslexia". Developmental Neuropsychology 35 (5): 475–493. doi:10.1080/87565641.2010.494748. PMID 20721770. http://pdfserve.informaworld.com/260009__925867416.pdf. 
  40. Fatemi, S. Hossein; Sartorius, Norman; Clayton, Paula J. (2008). The Medical Basis of Psychiatry (3 ed.). Springer Science & Business Media. p. 308. ISBN 978-1-59745-252-6. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109101234/https://books.google.com/books?id=RJOy1vy2RKQC&pg=PA308. 
  41. Capellini, Simone Aparecida (2007). Neuropsycholinguistic Perspectives on Dyslexia and Other Learning Disabilities. Nova Publishers. p. 94. ISBN 978-1-60021-537-7. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109113545/https://books.google.com/books?id=uiEaMQVwyzYC&pg=PA94. 
  42. Moore, D. R. (July 2011). "The diagnosis and management of auditory processing disorder". Lang Speech Hear Serv SCH 42 (3): 303–8. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0032). PMID 21757566. 
  43. 43.0 43.1 Pammer, Kristen (January 2014). "Brain mechanisms and reading remediation: more questions than answers.". Scientifica 2014: 802741. doi:10.1155/2014/802741. PMID 24527259. PMC 3913493. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3913493/. 
  44. Law, J (2014). "relationship of phonological ability, speech perception, and auditory perception in adults with dyslexia". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8: 482. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00482. PMID 25071512. PMC 4078926. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4078926/. 
  45. Susan J. Pickering (2012). "Chapter 2. Working Memory in Dyslexia". Working Memory and Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Psychology Press. ISBN 978-1-135-42134-2. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://books.google.com/books?id=IoXidOBdNpMC&pg=PA29. 
  46. 46.0 46.1 Berkhan O (1917). "Über die Wortblindheit, ein Stammeln im Sprechen und Schreiben, ein Fehl im Lesen". Neurologisches Centralblatt 36: 914–27. https://books.google.com/?id=DmEsAQAAIAAJ&dq=editions%3AUCALB3248710&q=Wortblindheit#search_anchor. 
  47. Reid, Gavin; Fawcett, Angela; Manis, Frank; Siegel, Linda (2008). The SAGE Handbook of Dyslexia. SAGE Publications. p. 127. ISBN 978-1-84860-037-9. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109200307/https://books.google.com/books?id=937rqz4Ryc8C&pg=PA127. 
  48. 48.0 48.1 Stein, John (2014). "Dyslexia: the Role of Vision and Visual Attention". Current Developmental Disorders Reports 1 (4): 267–80. doi:10.1007/s40474-014-0030-6. PMID 25346883. PMC 4203994. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4203994/. 
  49. Whitaker, Harry A. (2010). Concise Encyclopedia of Brain and Language. Elsevier. p. 180. ISBN 978-0-08-096499-7. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109173223/https://books.google.com/books?id=GNcDiRV2jJQC&pg=PA180. 
  50. {{cite journal|last1=Price|first1=cathy|title=A Review and Synthesis of the first 20 years of Pet and fMRI studies of heard Speech, Spoken Language and Reading|journal=NeuroImage|date=16 August 2012|volume=62 |issue=2|pages=816–847|doi=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.062|pmid=22584224|}
  51. Sharifi, S (May 2014). "Neuroimaging essentials in essential tremor: a systematic review.". NeuroImage: Clinical 5: 217–231. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2014.05.003. PMID 25068111. PMC 4110352. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4110352/. 
  52. Brandler, William (February 2014). "The genetic relationship between handedness and neurodevelopmental disorders". Trends in Molecular Medicine 20 (2): 83–90. doi:10.1016/j.molmed.2013.10.008. PMID 24275328. PMC 3969300. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3969300/. 
  53. Cain, Kate (2010). Reading development and difficulties (1st ed.). TJ International. p. 134. ISBN 9781405151559. https://books.google.com/?id=FT6RALjOr9QC&pg=PA134&dq=cerebellar+theory+of+dyslexia#v=onepage&q=cerebellar%20theory%20of%20dyslexia&f=false. Retrieved 21 March 2015. 
  54. Levav, Itzhak (2009). Psychiatric and Behavioral Disorders in Israel: From Epidemiology to Mental health. Green Publishing. p. 52. ISBN 9789652294685. https://books.google.com/?id=W2RzffMnpg8C&pg=PA52&dq=cerebellar+theory+of+dyslexia#v=onepage&q=cerebellar%20theory%20of%20dyslexia&f=false. Retrieved 21 March 2015. 
  55. Faust, Miriam (2012). The Handbook of the Neuropsychology of Language. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 941–43. ISBN 978-1-4443-3040-3. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109200538/https://books.google.com/books?id=UEWVqdNFL4cC&pg=PA941. 
  56. Benitez, A (November 2010). "Neurobiology and neurogenetics of dyslexia". Neurology (In Spanish) 25 (9): 563–81. doi:10.1016/j.nrl.2009.12.010. PMID 21093706. 
  57. Kere, Julia (September 2014). "The molecular genetics and neurobiology of developmental dyslexia as model of a complex phenotype". Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications 452 (2): 236–43. doi:10.1016/j.bbrc.2014.07.102. PMID 25078623. 
  58. Marshall, Chloë R. (2012). Current Issues in Developmental Disorders. Psychology Press. pp. 53–56. ISBN 978-1-136-23067-7. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109103320/https://books.google.com/books?id=jHqYP39rI40C&pg=PA53. 
  59. Rosen, Glenn D. (2013). The Dyslexic Brain: New Pathways in Neuroscience Discovery. Psychology Press. p. 342. ISBN 978-1-134-81550-0. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109143349/https://books.google.com/books?id=ZHBxBEekGSkC&pg=PA342. 
  60. Friend, A; Defries, J. C.; Olson, R. K. (November 2008). "Parental Education Moderates Genetic Influences on Reading Disability". Psychol. Sci. 19 (11): 1124–30. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02213.x. PMID 19076484. PMC 2605635. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2605635/. 
  61. Taylor, J.; Roehrig, A. D.; Hensler, B. Soden; Connor, C. M.; Schatschneider, C. (2010). "Teacher Quality Moderates the Genetic Effects on Early Reading". Science 328 (5977): 512–4. doi:10.1126/science.1186149. PMID 20413504. PMC 2905841. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2905841/. 
  62. 62.0 62.1 Pennington, Bruce F.; McGrath, Lauren M.; Rosenberg, Jenni; Barnard, Holly; Smith, Shelley D.; Willcutt, Erik G.; Friend, Angela; Defries, John C. et al. (January 2009). "Gene × Environment Interactions in Reading Disability and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder". Developmental Psychology 45 (1): 77–89. doi:10.1037/a0014549. PMID 19209992. PMC 2743891. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2743891/. 
  63. Roth, Tania L.; Roth, Eric D.; Sweatt, J. David (September 2010). "Epigenetic regulation of genes in learning and memory". Essays in Biochemistry 48 (1): 263–74. doi:10.1042/bse0480263. PMID 20822498. 
  64. Smith, Shelley D. (2011-12). "Approach to epigenetic analysis in language disorders". Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 3 (4): 356–364. doi:10.1007/s11689-011-9099-y. ISSN 1866-1947. PMID 22113455. PMC 3261263. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3261263/. 
  65. 65.0 65.1 65.2 Paulesu, Eraldo; Brunswick, Nicola and Paganelli, Federica (2010). "Cross-cultural differences in unimpaired and dyslexic reading: Behavioral and functional anatomical observations in readers of regular and irregular orthographies. Chapter 12 in Reading and Dyslexia in Different Orthographies Archived 9 January 2017 at the Wayback Machine. Eds. Nicola Brunswick, Siné McDougall, and Paul de Mornay Davies. Psychology Press. ISBN 9781135167813
  66. Juel, Connie (2013). "The Impact of Early School Experiences on Initial Reading". Handbook of Early Literacy Research. Guilford Publications. ISBN 978-1-4625-1470-0. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://books.google.com/books?id=_chXAQAAQBAJ&pg=PA421. 
  67. Snowling, Margaret J; Hulme, Charles (1 May 2012). "Annual Research Review: The nature and classification of reading disorders – a commentary on proposals for DSM-5". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines 53 (5): 593–607. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02495.x. PMID 22141434. PMC 3492851. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3492851/. 
  68. 68.0 68.1 Habib, Michael (2013). "Dyslexia". Pediatric Neurology Part I (in en). Handbook of Clinical Neurology. 111. pp. 229–235. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-52891-9.00023-3. ISBN 9780444528919. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780444528919000233. Retrieved 19 December 2018. 
  69. Schumacher, Johannes; Hoffmann, Per; Schmäl, Christine; Schulte‐Körne, Gerd; Nöthen, Markus M (2007). "Genetics of dyslexia: the evolving landscape". Journal of Medical Genetics 44 (5): 289–297. doi:10.1136/jmg.2006.046516. PMID 17307837. PMC 2597981. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2597981/. 
  70. 70.0 70.1 Pritchard SC, Coltheart M, Palethorpe S, Castles A; Coltheart; Palethorpe; Castles (October 2012). "Nonword reading: comparing dual-route cascaded and connectionist dual-process models with human data". J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 38 (5): 1268–88. doi:10.1037/a0026703. PMID 22309087. 
  71. Eysenck, Michael; Keane, Mark T. (2013). Cognitive Psychology 6e. Psychology Press. p. 373. ISBN 978-1-134-44046-7. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109123837/https://books.google.com/books?id=U-IF8PAa_jIC&pg=PA373. 
  72. 72.0 72.1 Eysenck, Michael; Keane, Mark T. (2013). Cognitive Psychology 6e. Psychology Press. p. 450. ISBN 978-1-134-44046-7. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109170422/https://books.google.com/books?id=U-IF8PAa_jIC&pg=PA450. 
  73. Hulme, Charles; Joshi, R. Malatesha; Snowling, Margaret J. (2012). Reading and Spelling: Development and Disorders. Routledge. p. 151. ISBN 978-1-136-49807-7. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109141419/https://books.google.com/books?id=MumCCKK4JR8C&pg=PT151. 
  74. Sprenger-Charolles, Liliane (2011). "Prevalence and Reliability of Phonological, Surface, and Mixed Profiles in Dyslexia: A Review of Studies Conducted in Languages Varying in Orthographic Depth". Scientific Studies of Reading 15 (6): 498–521. doi:10.1080/10888438.2010.524463. Archived from the original on 30 August 2017. https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00733553. 
  75. "Tests for Dyslexia and Learning Disabilities". University of Michigan. Archived from the original on 13 March 2015. Retrieved 15 March 2015. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (help)
  76. Peer, Lindsay; Reid, Gavin (2013). Introduction to Dyslexia. Taylor & Francis. pp. 35–40. ISBN 978-1-135-37290-3. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109134343/https://books.google.com/books?id=OTiAAAAAQBAJ&pg=PT35. 
  77. 77.0 77.1 "Screening and assessment". British Dyslexia Association. Archived from the original on 30 March 2015. Retrieved 11 March 2015. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (help)
  78. Fletcher, Jack (2009). "Dyslexia: the evolution of a scientific concept". Journal of International Neuropsychology Society 15 (4): 501–508. doi:10.1017/S1355617709090900. PMID 19573267. PMC 3079378. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3079378/. 
  79. Schulte-Körne, Gerd (October 2010). "The Prevention, Diagnosis, and Treatment of Dyslexia". Deutsches Ärzteblatt International 107 (41): 718–727. doi:10.3238/arztebl.2010.0718. ISSN 1866-0452. PMID 21046003. PMC 2967798. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2967798/. 
  80. Mather, N., & Schneider, D. The use of intelligence tests in the diagnosis of specific reading disability.Goldstein, Sam; Princiotta, Dana; Naglieri, Jack A. (2014). Handbook of Intelligence: Evolutionary Theory, Historical Perspective, and Current Concepts (in en). Springer. pp. 415–434. ISBN 9781493915620. https://books.google.com/?id=ylzEBQAAQBAJ&printsec=frontcover&dq=Handbook+of+Intelligence:+Evolutionary+theory,+historical+perspective,+and+current+concepts#v=onepage&q=Handbook%20of%20Intelligence%3A%20Evolutionary%20theory%2C%20historical%20perspective%2C%20and%20current%20concepts&f=false. Retrieved 10 January 2019. 
  81. Collett, Brent R.; Ohan, Jeneva L.; Myers, Kathleen M. (1 September 2003). "Ten-Year Review of Rating Scales. V: Scales Assessing Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder". Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 42 (9): 1015–1037. doi:10.1097/01.CHI.0000070245.24125.B6. ISSN 0890-8567. PMID 12960702. https://jaacap.org/article/S0890-8567(09)60999-0/fulltext. Retrieved 3 October 2019. 
  82. Stone, Lisanne L; Janssens, Jan M A M; Vermulst, Ad A; Van Der Maten, Marloes; Engels, Rutger C M E; Otten, Roy (20 February 2015). "The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: psychometric properties of the parent and teacher version in children aged 4–7". BMC Psychology 3 (1): 4. doi:10.1186/s40359-015-0061-8. ISSN 2050-7283. PMID 25815194. PMC 4364334. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4364334/. 
  83. Swart, G. T. (NaN). "The Clinician's Guide To The Behavior Assessment System For Children". The Canadian Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Review 14 (3): 90. ISSN 1716-9119. PMC 2542918. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2542918/. 
  84. Birmaher, B.; Khetarpal, S.; Brent, D.; Cully, M.; Balach, L.; Kaufman, J.; Neer, S. M. (NaN). "The Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED): scale construction and psychometric characteristics". Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 36 (4): 545–553. doi:10.1097/00004583-199704000-00018. ISSN 0890-8567. PMID 9100430. 
  85. Lindquist, E. F. (1953). The Iowa tests of educational development: how to use the test results; a manual for teachers and counselors (in en). Science Research Associates. https://books.google.com/?id=yOc9AAAAYAAJ&q=Iowa+Tests+of+Educational+Development&dq=Iowa+Tests+of+Educational+Development. Retrieved 3 October 2019. 
  86. Dell, Cindy Ann; Harrold, Barbara; Dell, Thomas (1 October 2008). "Test Review: Wilkinson, G. S., & Robertson, G. J. (2006). Wide Range Achievement Test—Fourth Edition. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. WRAT4 Introductory Kit (includes manual, 25 test/response forms [blue and green, and accompanying test materials): $243.00"]. Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin 52 (1): 57–60. doi:10.1177/0034355208320076. ISSN 0034-3552. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/0034355208320076. Retrieved 4 October 2019. 
  87. Semrud-Clikeman, Margaret; Ellison, Phyllis Anne Teeter (2009). Child Neuropsychology: Assessment and Interventions for Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 2nd Edition (in en). Springer Science & Business Media. p. 119. ISBN 9780387889634. https://books.google.com/books?id=NBGSF9Jyg6AC&pg=PT119#v=onepage&q&f=false. Retrieved 3 October 2019. 
  88. "ICD-10 Version:2015". apps.who.int. Archived from the original on 2 November 2015. Retrieved 4 November 2015. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (help)
  89. "ICD-10 Version:2015". apps.who.int. Archived from the original on 2 November 2015. Retrieved 4 November 2015. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (help)
  90. "Specific learning disorders" (PDF). American Psychiatric Publishing. 2013. Archived from the original (PDF) on 23 September 2015. Retrieved 6 November 2015. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (help)
  91. Doyle, James (2008). Dyslexia: An Introduction Guide (in en). John Wiley & Sons. p. 92. ISBN 9780470698495. https://books.google.com/?id=CoPckyxP_CsC&pg=PA92&lpg=PA92&dq=dyslexia+types#v=onepage&q=dyslexia%20types&f=false. Retrieved 22 December 2018. 
  92. Fletcher, Jack M.; Lyon, G. Reid; Fuchs, Lynn S.; Barnes, Marcia A. (2006). Learning Disabilities, First Edition: From Identification to Intervention (in en). Guilford Press. pp. 99–100. ISBN 9781606238141. https://books.google.com/?id=9MbEe-L15UYC&printsec=frontcover&dq=Fletcher,+J.+M.,+Lyon,+G.+R.,+Fuchs,+L.+S.,+%26+Barnes,+M.+A.+(2007).+Learning+disabilities:+From+identification+to+intervention.+New+York:+Guilford.#v=snippet&q=dyslexia%20subtype%20&f=false. 
  93. Verga, Laura (September 2013). "How relevant is social interaction in second language learning". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7: 550. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00550. PMID 24027521. PMC 3759854. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3759854/. 
  94. Wilson, Stephen (March 2013). "Dysfunctional visual word form processing in progressive alexia". Brain 136 (4): 1260–73. doi:10.1093/brain/awt034. PMID 23471694. PMC 3613714. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3613714/. 
  95. Johnson, Juliene (April 2013). "Jean-Martin Charcot's role in the 19th century study of music aphasia". Brain 136 (5): 1662–70. doi:10.1093/brain/awt055. PMID 23576129. 
  96. Harley, Trevor (2014). The Psychology of Language: From Data to Theory (4th ed.). Psychology Press. p. 227. ISBN 9781317938439. https://books.google.com/?id=Ax5iAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA227&dq=dual+route#v=onepage&q=dual%20route&f=false. Retrieved 14 February 2015. 
  97. Wandell, Brian (January 2012). "Learning to See Words". Annual Review of Psychology 63: 31–53. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100434. PMID 21801018. PMC 3228885. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3228885/. 
  98. Vandermosten, Maaike (2012). "tractography study in dyslexia: neuroanatomic correlates of orthographic, phonological and speech processing". Brain 135 (3): 935–948. doi:10.1093/brain/awr363. PMID 22327793. 
  99. Ayers, Susan; Baum, Andrew; McManus, Chris; Newman, Stanton; Wallston, Kenneth; Weinman, John; West, Robert (2007). Cambridge Handbook of Psychology, Health and Medicine. Cambridge University Press. p. 689. ISBN 978-1-139-46526-7. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109122027/https://books.google.com/books?id=hNY7P1z6qBoC&pg=PA689. 
  100. Heim, Stefan; Wehnelt, Anke; Grande, Marion; Huber, Walter; Amunts, Katrin (May 2013). "Effects of lexicality and word frequency on brain activation in dyslexic readers". Brain and Language 125 (2): 194–202. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2011.12.005. PMID 22230039. 
  101. Mather, Nancy; Wendling, Barbara J. (2011). Essentials of Dyslexia Assessment and Intervention. John Wiley & Sons. p. 272. ISBN 978-1-118-15266-9. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109095150/https://books.google.com/books?id=k6Gg-zMYv14C&pg=PA272. 
  102. Beeson, Pélagie M.; Rising, Kindle; Kim, Esther S.; Rapcsak, Steven Z. (April 2010). "A Treatment Sequence for Phonological Alexia/Agraphia". Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 53 (2): 450–68. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0229). PMID 20360466. PMC 3522177. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3522177/. 
  103. Friederici, Angela D (June 2018). "The neural basis for human syntax: Broca's area and beyond". Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 21: 88–92. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.03.004. ISSN 2352-1546. 
  104. Binder, Jeffrey R. (15 December 2015). "The Wernicke area". Neurology 85 (24): 2170–2175. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000002219. ISSN 0028-3878. PMID 26567270. PMC 4691684. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4691684/. 
  105. Snowling, Margaret J.; Stackhouse, Joy (2013-05-21). Dyslexia, Speech and Language : a Practitioner's Handbook. (in en) (2nd ed.). Wiley. ISBN 9781118687758. https://books.google.com/?id=v0bguE61uJMC&pg=PT48&dq=wernicke's+area+dyslexia#v=onepage&q=wernicke's%20area%20dyslexia&f=false. Retrieved 18 December 2018. 
  106. Tonkonogy, Joseph M.; Puente, Antonio E. (2009). Localization of Clinical Syndromes in Neuropsychology and Neuroscience. Springer Publishing Company. p. 409. ISBN 978-0-8261-1968-1. Archived from the original on 29 April 2016. https://web.archive.org/web/20160429201951/https://books.google.com/books?id=t2Y4gv9d3l0C&pg=PA409. 
  107. Leff, Alexander; Starrfelt, Randi (17 October 2013). Alexia: Diagnosis, Treatment and Theory. Springer Science & Business Media. ISBN 9781447155294. Archived from the original on 6 September 2015. https://web.archive.org/web/20150906073812/https://books.google.com/books?id=Yva6BAAAQBAJ. 
  108. Rapcsak, Steven Z.; Beeson, Pélagie M.; Henry, Maya L.; Leyden, Anne; Kim, Esther; Rising, Kindle; Andersen, Sarah; Cho, HyeSuk (1 January 2009). "Phonological dyslexia and dysgraphia: Cognitive mechanisms and neural substrates". Cortex 45 (5): 575–91. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2008.04.006. PMID 18625494. PMC 2689874. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2689874/. 
  109. 109.0 109.1 Ellis, Andrew W.; Young, Andrew W. (22 August 2013). Human Cognitive Neuropsychology (Classic Edition). Psychology Press. ISBN 9781135078478. Archived from the original on 6 September 2015. https://web.archive.org/web/20150906080616/https://books.google.com/books?id=rX5_AAAAQBAJ. 
  110. Yanoff, Myron (30 January 2014). Ophthalmic Diagnosis & Treatment. JP Medical Ltd. ISBN 9789350259528. Archived from the original on 6 September 2015. https://web.archive.org/web/20150906081234/https://books.google.com/books?id=lwfiAgAAQBAJ. 
  111. "Visual Field Defects. Free Medical Information | Patient". Patient. Archived from the original on 9 August 2015. Retrieved 23 August 2015. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (help)
  112. Brown, Thomas E. (2009). ADHD Comorbidities: Handbook for ADHD Complications in Children and Adults. American Psychiatric Pub. p. 236. ISBN 978-1-58562-158-3. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109200434/https://books.google.com/books?id=tqfHS0xfF-IC&pg=PA236. 
  113. Starrfelt, Randi; Ólafsdóttir, Rannveig Rós; Arendt, Ida-Marie (2013). "Rehabilitation of pure alexia: A review". Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 23 (5): 755–79. doi:10.1080/09602011.2013.809661. PMID 23808895. PMC 3805423. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3805423/. 
  114. Hillis, Argye E. (2013). The Handbook of Adult Language Disorders. Psychology Press. p. 40. ISBN 978-1-134-94801-7. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109134359/https://books.google.com/books?id=CQqijCk4pv8C&pg=PA40. 
  115. Papathanasiou, Ilias; Coppens, Patrick; Potagas, Constantin (2012). Aphasia and Related Neurogenic Communication Disorders. Jones & Bartlett Publishers. p. 168. ISBN 978-1-4496-8435-8. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109135113/https://books.google.com/books?id=L2a57bE9RXQC&pg=PA168. 
  116. Boller, François; Grafman, Jordan (2010). Handbook of Neuropsychology: Disorders of visual behavior. Gulf Professional Publishing. p. 31. ISBN 978-0-444-50369-5. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109134312/https://books.google.com/books?id=8qywq48-sl8C&pg=PA31. 
  117. Schuett, Susanne (2009). "The rehabilitation of hemianopic dyslexia". Nature Reviews Neurology 5 (8): 427–37. doi:10.1038/nrneurol.2009.97. PMID 19581901. 
  118. Farah, Martha J.; Ratcliff, Graham (2013). The Neuropsychology of High-level Vision. Psychology Press. pp. 177–179. ISBN 978-1-135-80652-1. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109194707/https://books.google.com/books?id=9m-SJD_K7B0C&pg=PA177. 
  119. Comer, Ronald (2011). Psychology Around Us. RR Donnelley. pp. 232. ISBN 978-0-471-38519-6. 
  120. Bogon, Johana (October 2014). "TVA based assessment of visual attention functions in developmental dyslexia". Frontiers in Psychology 5: 1172. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01172. PMID 25360129. PMC 4199262. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4199262/. 
  121. Brunswick, Nicola (10 April 2012). Supporting Dyslexic Adults in Higher Education and the Workplace. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 115–. ISBN 978-0-470-97479-7. Archived from the original on 31 December 2013. https://web.archive.org/web/20131231081312/http://books.google.com/books?id=suc1o0hueowC&pg=PA115. Retrieved 10 April 2012. 
  122. 122.0 122.1 Schulte-Körne, G (October 2010). "The prevention, diagnosis, and treatment of dyslexia". Deutsches Ärzteblatt International 107 (41): 718–26. doi:10.3238/arztebl.2010.0718. PMID 21046003. PMC 2967798. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2967798/. 
  123. Lyytinen, Heikki; Erskine, Jane; Aro, Mikko and Richardson, Ulla (2009). "Reading and reading disorders". In Hoff, Erika. Blackwell Handbook of Language Development. Blackwell. pp. 454–474. ISBN 978-1-4051-9459-4. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109204820/https://books.google.com/books?id=PCy6c9hIL5YC&pg=PA454. 
  124. van der Leij, Aryan (1 November 2013). "Dyslexia and early intervention: what did we learn from the Dutch Dyslexia Programme?". Dyslexia (Chichester, England) 19 (4): 241–255. doi:10.1002/dys.1466. ISSN 1099-0909. PMID 24133037. 
  125. 125.0 125.1 de Leeuw, Renske (December 2010). Special Font For Dyslexia? (in English, Dutch). University of Twente. p. 32. Archived from the original on 1 November 2011. http://www.ilo.gw.utwente.nl/ilo/attachments/032_Masterthesis_Leeuw.pdf. 
  126. Sawers, Paul (30 June 2011). "Dyslexie: A typeface for dyslexics". Archived from the original on 13 April 2012. Retrieved 9 April 2012. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (help)
  127. 127.0 127.1 Marinus, E; Mostard, M; Segers, E; Schubert, TM; Madelaine, A; Wheldall, K (August 2016). "A Special Font for People with Dyslexia: Does it Work and, if so, why?". Dyslexia (Chichester, England) 22 (3): 233–44. doi:10.1002/dys.1527. PMID 27194598. 
  128. Barquero, Laura (2014). "Neuroimaging of reading intervention". PLoS ONE 9 (1): e83668. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0083668. PMID 24427278. PMC 3888398. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3888398/. 
  129. Cogo-Moreira, Hugo; Andriolo, Régis B; Yazigi, Latife; Ploubidis, George B; Brandão de Ávila, Clara Regina; Mari, Jair J (15 August 2012). "Music education for improving reading skills in children and adolescents with dyslexia". Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (8): CD009133. doi:10.1002/14651858.cd009133.pub2. PMID 22895983. 
  130. O’Hare, Anne (2010). "Dyslexia: what do paediatricians need to know?". Pediatrics and Child Health 20 (7): 338–343. doi:10.1016/j.paed.2010.04.004. 
  131. "Response to Intervention Guidance - Minimum Requirements of a Response to Intervention Program (RtI) - Instruction Matched to Student Need: Special Education : P12 : NYSED". www.p12.nysed.gov. Retrieved 10 January 2019.
  132. Tasman, Allan; Kay, Jerald; Lieberman, Jeffrey A.; First, Michael B.; Riba, Michelle (29 January 2015). Psychiatry, 2 Volume Set. John Wiley & Sons. ISBN 9781118845493. Archived from the original on 6 September 2015. https://web.archive.org/web/20150906081853/https://books.google.com/books?id=6Rp0BgAAQBAJ. 
  133. Protopapas, Athanassios (2013). "From temporal processing to developmental language disorders: mind the gap". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 369 (1634): 20130090. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0090. PMID 24324245. PMC 3866431. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3866431/. 
  134. Zhao, Jing (November 2014). "The visual magnocellular-dorsal dysfunction in Chinese children with developmental dyslexia impedes Chinese character recognition.". Scientific Reports 4: 7068. doi:10.1038/srep07068. PMID 25412386. PMC 4238300. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4238300/. 
  135. Marshall, Chloe (2012). Current Issues in Developmental Disorders. Psychology Press. p. 152. ISBN 978-1-84872-084-8. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109144200/https://books.google.com/books?id=5VK_TWsQ3N4C&pg=PA152. 
  136. Garralda, Elena; Raynaud, Jean-Philippe (16 January 2012). Brain, Mind, and Developmental Psychopathology in Childhood. Jason Aronson. ISBN 9780765708663. https://books.google.com/?id=5ujeVaMa9U0C&pg=PA177. 
  137. Navas, Ana Luiza Gomes Pinto; Ferraz, Érica de Cássia; Borges, Juliana Postigo Amorina; Navas, Ana Luiza Gomes Pinto; Ferraz, Érica de Cássia; Borges, Juliana Postigo Amorina (2014). "Phonological processing deficits as a universal model for dyslexia: evidence from different orthographies". CoDAS 26 (6): 509–519. doi:10.1590/2317-1782/20142014135. PMID 25590915. 
  138. "Über Dyslexie". Archiv für Psychiatrie 15: 276–278. 1884. 
  139. Annual of the Universal Medical Sciences and Analytical Index: A Yearly Report of the Progress of the General Sanitary Sciences Throughout the World. F. A. Davis Company. 1888. p. 39. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109200623/https://books.google.com/books?id=5_IhAQAAMAAJ&pg=PA39. 
  140. Brooks, Patricia (2014). Encyclopedia of language development. SAGE. p. 30. ISBN 9781483346434. https://books.google.com/?id=mvfSAwAAQBAJ&pg=PR30. 
  141. Mishra, Srikanta K. (October 2014). "Medial efferent mechanisms in children with auditory processing disorders.". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8: 860. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00860. PMID 25386132. PMC 4209830. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4209830/. 
  142. Reid, Gavin (2012). The Routledge Companion to Dyslexia. Routledge. p. 16. ISBN 978-1-136-61710-2. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. https://web.archive.org/web/20170109205019/https://books.google.com/books?id=QrBQAmfXYooC&pg=PA16. 
  143. Richlan, Fabio (May 2014). "Functional neuroanatomy of developmental dyslexia; the role of orthographic depth". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8: 347. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00347. PMID 24904383. PMC 4033006. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4033006/. 
  144. "Reading Difficulty and Disability" (PDF). report.nih.gov. NIH. Retrieved 10 January 2019.
  145. de Berr, J (2014). "Factors influencing work participation of adults with developmental dyslexia". BMC Public Health 14: 77. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-14-77. PMID 24460949. PMC 3913008. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3913008/. 
  146. Pino, Marco; Mortari, Luigina (1 November 2014). "The Inclusion of Students with Dyslexia in Higher Education: A Systematic Review Using Narrative Synthesis". Dyslexia (Chichester, England) 20 (4): 346–369. doi:10.1002/dys.1484. PMID 25293652. PMC 4253321. //www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4253321/. 

Further reading